Loftus and Palmer 1974 Car Crash Experiment

Loftus and Palmer Car Crash Study (1974)

Key Takeaways

  • Memory malleability: Loftus & Palmer demonstrated that memories are not fixed recordings but reconstructions that can be altered by subsequent information, questioning techniques, and suggestion.
  • Misinformation effect: Their research proved that post-event information can distort eyewitness memory, with participants recalling higher speeds and nonexistent broken glass when questions contained suggestive verbs like “smashed.”
  • False memory creation: They showed that entirely false memories can be implanted through suggestion, with about 25% of participants developing detailed memories of events that never occurred.
  • Legal impact: Their findings revolutionized the legal system’s approach to eyewitness testimony, leading to reforms in police questioning techniques and judicial guidelines for evaluating witness evidence.

Introduction

Elizabeth Loftus stands as one of the most influential psychologists in the field of memory research. Her groundbreaking work on eyewitness testimony and the malleability of human memory has transformed our understanding of how memories are formed, stored, and recalled. Since the 1970s, her research has demonstrated that memories are not fixed recordings of events but rather reconstructions that can be altered by subsequent information.

Loftus’s work has had profound implications across multiple disciplines. In psychology, she challenged prevailing notions about memory reliability. In the legal system, her research raised serious questions about the validity of eyewitness testimony. In clinical settings, her findings on false memories reshaped therapeutic approaches to trauma and repression.

This article examines Loftus’s key theories, her most significant research, and her lasting impact on psychology and beyond.

Elizabeth Loftus: Background and Career

Elizabeth F. Loftus was born in 1944 in Los Angeles, California. She earned her undergraduate degree in mathematics and psychology from UCLA and her Ph.D. in psychology from Stanford University in 1970. Initially focusing on mathematical psychology, Loftus soon shifted her research interests to human memory.

Her early career at the University of Washington saw her begin the research on eyewitness testimony that would define her professional life. In 2002, she joined the faculty at the University of California, Irvine, where she continues to work as Distinguished Professor in the Department of Psychological Science and the Department of Criminology, Law, and Society.

Throughout her career, Loftus has received numerous accolades, including the Gold Medal Award for Life Achievement in the Science of Psychology from the American Psychological Foundation and election to the National Academy of Sciences. She has authored over 20 books and published hundreds of scientific articles, establishing herself as the foremost authority on memory distortion and eyewitness testimony.

Loftus Core Theories and Research

The Malleability of Memory

At the heart of Loftus’s work is the concept that memory is not a perfect recording of events but a reconstructive process. Unlike a video camera that captures events exactly as they occur, human memory constantly evolves through a process of encoding, storage, and retrieval—each step susceptible to distortion.

Loftus demonstrated that memories can be altered by information encountered after the original event. This finding directly challenged earlier views of memory as a stable, fixed entity and established the foundation for understanding memory as malleable and reconstructive.

Post-Event Information Effect

One of Loftus’s most significant contributions is her research on how post-event information can alter memory. Through a series of experiments, she established that when people witness an event and then receive new information about that event, their recollection often incorporates the new information—even when it contradicts what they actually saw.

This effect is particularly strong when:

  • The original memory is weak or unclear
  • The post-event information comes from a source perceived as authoritative
  • Significant time has passed between the event and recall
  • The post-event information seems plausible within the context of the original event

The Misinformation Effect

Building on her work with post-event information, Loftus formally defined the “misinformation effect”—the phenomenon where memory becomes less accurate due to exposure to misleading information after the event. Her research demonstrated that even subtle changes in how questions are phrased can substantially alter what people remember.

In her famous car crash experiment with John Palmer (1974), participants viewed films of automobile accidents and then answered questions about what they had seen. The critical manipulation was in the wording of the questions. When asked, “How fast were the cars going when they smashed into each other?” participants estimated higher speeds than when the verb “hit” was used. Moreover, when tested a week later, those in the “smashed” condition were more likely to falsely remember seeing broken glass, which was never present in the original films.

This experiment and others demonstrated that the very language used to question witnesses could implant false information that becomes integrated into their memory of the event.

False Memory Research

Perhaps Loftus’s most controversial and influential work involves false memories—entirely fabricated recollections of events that never occurred. In a series of studies in the 1990s, Loftus and her colleagues demonstrated that it was possible to implant entirely false memories in approximately 25% of participants.

In the “Lost in the Mall” study, participants were given written accounts of four childhood events, three true and one false (being lost in a shopping mall). After being encouraged to remember these events, a significant number of participants not only “remembered” the false event but embellished it with vivid sensory details.

This research raised profound questions about the reliability of recovered memories in therapeutic settings, particularly those involving childhood abuse. It suggested that well-intentioned therapeutic techniques might inadvertently create false memories rather than recover repressed ones.

Weapon Focus Effect

Loftus also identified the “weapon focus effect,” where the presence of a weapon during a crime leads witnesses to focus their attention on the weapon rather than other details like the perpetrator’s face. This attentional narrowing helps explain why eyewitness identifications in armed robberies and similar crimes are often unreliable.

Through this diverse body of research, Loftus established a comprehensive framework for understanding memory distortion that continues to inform psychology, law, and other fields today.

Loftus & Palmer Car Crash Experiment (1974)

The 1974 car crash experiment conducted with John Palmer stands as one of Loftus’s most influential studies and perfectly illustrates her approach to memory research.

Methodology

The experiment consisted of two parts:

Experiment One:

  • 45 students from the University of Washington participated
  • Participants viewed seven film clips of traffic accidents
  • After each clip, they answered questions about what they had seen
  • The critical question varied the verb used: “How fast were the cars going when they ___ each other?” with the blank filled by either “smashed,” “collided,” “bumped,” “hit,” or “contacted”
  • This was an independent measures design with the verb serving as the independent variable and the estimated speed as the dependent variable

Experiment Two:

  • 150 students participated in a follow-up study
  • They watched a one-minute film of a multiple-car accident
  • Participants were divided into three groups:
    • One group was asked about the cars “smashing” into each other
    • One group was asked about the cars “hitting” each other
    • A control group received no questions about speed
  • One week later, all participants answered questions about the accident, including whether they had seen broken glass (none was present in the original film)

Key Findings

The results clearly demonstrated the power of language to alter memory:

Experiment One Results:

Verb UsedAverage Estimated Speed (mph)
“Smashed”40.8
“Collided”39.3
“Bumped”38.1
“Hit”34.0
“Contacted”31.8
Loftus and Palmer Car Crash Study (1974)

Experiment Two Results:

Response“Smashed” Group“Hit” GroupControl Group
Saw broken glass1676
Didn’t see broken glass344344

As shown in these tables, participants who were asked about cars “smashing” were more than twice as likely to report seeing broken glass compared to those in the “hit” condition or control group, despite no broken glass being present in the original film.

Implications

The study demonstrated two critical aspects of memory:

  1. Response Bias: The wording of questions can create a bias in how people respond, without necessarily changing their underlying memory.
  2. Memory Alteration: More profoundly, the study suggested that leading questions could actually change the memory representation itself, causing people to “remember” details (like broken glass) that were never present.

This landmark study helped establish that eyewitness testimony, far from being a reliable record of events, is highly susceptible to suggestion and can be manipulated through simple linguistic devices.

Applications of Loftus’s Research

Loftus’s research has revolutionized how the legal system approaches eyewitness testimony. Her findings have led to:

  • Changes in how police conduct lineups and interviews with witnesses
  • Judicial guidelines for evaluating the reliability of eyewitness evidence
  • The increasing use of expert witnesses to educate juries about memory limitations
  • Appeals and exonerations in cases that relied heavily on eyewitness testimony

The 1976 Devlin Report in the UK, influenced by Loftus’s work, recommended that judges warn juries about the potential unreliability of eyewitness testimony, establishing that convictions should not rest on such evidence alone.

Forensic Interview Techniques

Loftus’s research directly influenced the development of the Cognitive Interview technique, which aims to maximize accurate recall while minimizing suggestion. This approach emphasizes:

  • Open-ended rather than leading questions
  • Allowing witnesses to report events in their own words
  • Multiple retrieval attempts from different perspectives
  • Recreating the context of the original event

These techniques are now standard practice in many police departments and investigative agencies worldwide.

Clinical Applications

In clinical psychology, Loftus’s work on false memories has led to a reevaluation of certain therapeutic techniques, particularly those aimed at recovering repressed memories. Her research suggests caution when:

  • Using hypnosis or guided imagery to recover memories
  • Repeatedly questioning clients about potential past abuse
  • Interpreting dreams or symptoms as evidence of repressed trauma

While controversial in some therapeutic circles, these insights have helped establish more evidence-based approaches to trauma treatment.

Critical Evaluation of Loftus’s Work

Strengths

Loftus’s research exhibits several notable strengths:

  • Methodological rigor: Her experiments are carefully designed with appropriate controls and clearly operationalized variables.
  • Replicability: Many of her findings have been consistently replicated across different laboratories and contexts.
  • Real-world relevance: Unlike many psychological theories, her work has direct, practical applications in legal and clinical settings.
  • Interdisciplinary impact: Few psychologists have influenced fields as diverse as law, clinical practice, and cognitive science.

Limitations

Critics have identified several limitations:

  • Ecological validity: Laboratory studies may not fully capture the emotional impact and complexity of real-world traumatic events.
  • Ethical constraints: For obvious ethical reasons, researchers cannot experimentally implant traumatic false memories, limiting the generalizability of findings to severe trauma cases.
  • Sample bias: Many studies used college students, who may not be representative of the general population in memory functioning or suggestibility.
  • Conflicting research: Studies by Yuille and Cutshall (1986) found that witnesses to real violent crimes were relatively resistant to misleading information, suggesting laboratory findings might overestimate memory malleability in high-stress, real-world situations.

Comparison with Other Memory Theorists

Bartlett’s Schema Theory

Frederic Bartlett’s schema theory (1932) provides an interesting precursor to Loftus’s work. Bartlett demonstrated that recall is influenced by existing knowledge structures or “schemas,” showing how cultural expectations cause systematic distortions in memory. While Bartlett focused on how internal schemas affect memory, Loftus emphasized external sources of distortion through post-event information.

Daniel Schacter’s Seven Sins of Memory

Daniel Schacter’s framework of memory’s “seven sins” (transience, absent-mindedness, blocking, misattribution, suggestibility, bias, and persistence) integrates well with Loftus’s research, which primarily addresses suggestibility and misattribution. Schacter’s approach provides a broader taxonomy of memory failures, while Loftus offers deeper experimental evidence on specific mechanisms of distortion.

Integration with Neuroscience

Recent neuroscientific research has provided biological support for Loftus’s behavioral findings. Brain imaging studies show that recalling a memory makes it temporarily malleable before reconsolidation, providing a neural basis for the post-event information effect. This integration of cognitive and neuroscientific approaches has strengthened the theoretical foundation of memory malleability.

Controversies and Debates

The Recovered Memory Debate

Perhaps the most heated controversy surrounding Loftus’s work involves recovered memories of childhood abuse. During the 1980s and 1990s, some therapists claimed to help clients recover repressed memories of childhood trauma. Loftus’s research on false memories challenged the validity of these recovered memories, suggesting they might be iatrogenically implanted rather than authentically recovered.

This position made Loftus a controversial figure in some clinical circles and led to intense debates about the nature of traumatic memory. The controversy highlighted the tension between supporting abuse survivors and maintaining scientific rigor in therapeutic practices.

Expert Testimony Controversies

As an expert witness in numerous high-profile cases, Loftus has faced criticism from those who believe her testimony favors defendants and potentially discredits victims. Her critics argue that laboratory findings may not apply to traumatic real-world events, while supporters maintain that scientific understanding of memory limitations is essential for just legal outcomes.

Ethical Considerations

Some ethicists have questioned the morality of implanting false memories in research participants, even with consent and debriefing. Others have raised concerns about how findings on memory malleability might be misused to discredit legitimate testimony in abuse cases.

Loftus has generally responded to these controversies by emphasizing scientific evidence while acknowledging the complexity and sensitivity of memory issues, particularly in cases involving trauma.

Legacy and Influence

Elizabeth Loftus’s impact extends far beyond academic psychology:

  • Her work has been cited in thousands of court cases and has directly influenced judicial decisions and policy
  • The concept of memory malleability has entered public consciousness, changing how many people think about their own memories
  • Her research methods have established a template for studying the intersection of cognitive psychology and real-world applications
  • She has inspired generations of researchers to pursue questions about memory accuracy and distortion

Professionally, Loftus has received nearly every major award in psychology, including the National Medal of Science. More importantly, her work has helped prevent wrongful convictions and has encouraged more careful approaches to memory evidence in both legal and clinical contexts.

In 2002, the Review of General Psychology ranked Loftus as the 58th most cited psychologist of the 20th century, and the highest-ranking woman on the list—a testament to her extraordinary influence on the field.

Conclusion

Elizabeth Loftus’s half-century of research has fundamentally transformed our understanding of human memory. Far from being a faithful recording of experience, memory has been revealed as a reconstructive process vulnerable to suggestion, misinformation, and distortion. This insight represents one of the most significant paradigm shifts in modern psychology.

The practical implications of Loftus’s work cannot be overstated. In courtrooms, her research has led to more careful evaluation of eyewitness testimony and helped prevent wrongful convictions. In clinical settings, it has encouraged more evidence-based approaches to memory recovery. In everyday life, it has prompted healthy skepticism about the absolute reliability of our own recollections.

Loftus’s career exemplifies how rigorous experimental psychology can address questions of profound real-world significance. While laboratory studies of memory might seem removed from daily concerns, her work demonstrates how basic research can ultimately impact justice, clinical practice, and our fundamental understanding of human experience.

As memory research continues to evolve with new technologies like functional neuroimaging and advanced computational models, Loftus’s foundational insights remain relevant. The malleability of memory is now established as a fundamental principle in cognitive science, and future discoveries will likely build upon rather than replace her essential contributions.

Frequently Asked Questions

Who is Elizabeth Loftus?

Elizabeth Loftus is a cognitive psychologist and expert on human memory, particularly known for her groundbreaking research on the malleability of memory and eyewitness testimony. Born in 1944, she is currently a Distinguished Professor at the University of California, Irvine. Loftus has demonstrated how memories can be altered by suggestion, leading to profound impacts on the legal system and psychology. Her work has challenged the reliability of eyewitness testimony and revealed how false memories can be implanted. She ranks among the most influential psychologists of the 20th century.

What is the misinformation effect?

The misinformation effect, discovered by Elizabeth Loftus, describes how a person’s memory can become less accurate due to exposure to misleading information after an event. This effect demonstrates that memory is reconstructive rather than reproductive. In Loftus’s classic car crash experiment, simply changing a verb in a question (asking about cars that “smashed” rather than “hit”) led participants to remember higher speeds and even report seeing broken glass that wasn’t present. The misinformation effect is especially powerful when the original memory is weak, the misleading information comes from an authoritative source, or significant time has passed.

Elizabeth Loftus’s research has profoundly influenced the legal system by challenging the reliability of eyewitness testimony. Her work has led to reforms in how police conduct witness interviews and lineups, with emphasis on avoiding leading questions. Courts now often allow expert witnesses to educate juries about memory limitations, and judges may issue specific instructions about evaluating eyewitness evidence. The Devlin Report (1976), influenced by Loftus, recommended that judges warn juries about relying solely on eyewitness testimony for convictions. Her research has contributed to numerous case reviews and exonerations of wrongfully convicted individuals.

What is a false memory according to Loftus?

According to Elizabeth Loftus, a false memory is a recollection of an event that never actually occurred or a distorted recollection of a real event. Her research demonstrated that false memories can be implanted through suggestion, leading people to genuinely believe they experienced events that never happened. In her famous “Lost in the Mall” study, she showed that about 25% of participants could develop detailed false memories of being lost in a shopping mall as children when the suggestion was paired with real childhood memories. False memories feel authentic to the individual and can include sensory details, emotions, and contextual information despite having no basis in reality.

What is the weapon focus effect?

The weapon focus effect, identified by Elizabeth Loftus, occurs when the presence of a weapon during a crime causes witnesses to focus their attention on the weapon rather than other details like the perpetrator’s face or clothing. This attentional narrowing happens because weapons are both threatening and unexpected, triggering a stress response that affects how memories are encoded. As a result, eyewitness identifications in armed crimes are often less reliable than in crimes without weapons. This effect helps explain why witnesses to armed robberies may provide detailed descriptions of the gun but struggle to identify the robber in a lineup.

What was the “Lost in the Mall” study?

The “Lost in the Mall” study, conducted by Elizabeth Loftus and Jim Coan in the 1990s, demonstrated how false memories could be implanted in adults. Participants were given booklets containing four childhood stories—three true stories provided by family members and one false story about being lost in a shopping mall. After working with these stories over several days, approximately 25% of participants developed false memories of the mall incident, often adding sensory details and emotions not suggested in the original prompt. This influential study challenged assumptions about memory reliability and had significant implications for therapeutic practices involving recovered memories of childhood trauma.

What is the difference between the misinformation effect and false memories?

The misinformation effect and false memories represent different degrees of memory distortion in Loftus’s research. The misinformation effect involves the alteration of existing memories due to post-event information—like estimating higher speeds after hearing the word “smashed” instead of “hit” in the car crash experiment. False memories, by contrast, involve the creation of entirely new memories for events that never occurred—such as remembering being lost in a mall despite this never happening. While the misinformation effect typically modifies details of a real experience, false memories construct complete fabricated events that people genuinely believe they experienced, often with rich sensory and emotional components.

How has Loftus’s work on false memories been controversial?

Loftus’s work on false memories has been controversial primarily due to its implications for recovered memories of childhood abuse in therapeutic settings. During the 1980s-1990s, some therapists claimed to help clients recover repressed memories of abuse. Loftus’s research suggested these “recovered” memories might sometimes be false memories inadvertently implanted through suggestive therapeutic techniques. This positioned her against some clinical practitioners and abuse survivors, who felt her work might discredit legitimate trauma. As an expert witness in abuse cases, she faced criticism for potentially helping defendants. Despite controversy, her research has ultimately led to more evidence-based approaches to trauma and memory in therapy.

How accurate is eyewitness testimony according to Loftus?

According to Loftus’s extensive research, eyewitness testimony is far less accurate than commonly assumed. Her studies show that memory is reconstructive rather than reproductive, making it vulnerable to suggestion, leading questions, and post-event information. Factors that reduce accuracy include the presence of weapons (weapon focus effect), cross-racial identification difficulties, high stress during the event, and how questions are phrased during interviews. The passage of time between the event and recall also decreases accuracy. While eyewitnesses can be correct, Loftus’s work demonstrates that their confidence in their memories does not necessarily correlate with accuracy—a finding with profound implications for the justice system.

References

Yuille, J. C., & Cutshall, J. L. (1986). A case study of eyewitness memory of a crime. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71(2), 291-301.

Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge University Press.

Devlin, P. (1976). Report of the Committee on Evidence of Identification in Criminal Cases. Her Majesty’s Stationery Office.

Loftus, E. F. (1975). Leading questions and the eyewitness report. Cognitive Psychology, 7(4), 560-572.

Loftus, E. F. (1979). Eyewitness testimony. Harvard University Press.

Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48(5), 518-537.

Loftus, E. F., & Ketcham, K. (1994). The myth of repressed memory: False memories and allegations of sexual abuse. St. Martin’s Press.

Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13(5), 585-589.

Loftus, E. F., & Pickrell, J. E. (1995). The formation of false memories. Psychiatric Annals, 25(12), 720-725.

Schacter, D. L. (1999). The seven sins of memory: Insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 54(3), 182-203.

Further Reading and Research

Recommended Articles

  • Loftus, E. F. (2005). Planting misinformation in the human mind: A 30-year investigation of the malleability of memory. Learning & Memory, 12(4), 361-366.
  • Frenda, S. J., Nichols, R. M., & Loftus, E. F. (2011). Current issues and advances in misinformation research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(1), 20-23.
  • Howe, M. L., & Knott, L. M. (2015). The fallibility of memory in judicial processes: Lessons from the past and their modern consequences. Memory, 23(5), 633-656.

Suggested Books

  • Loftus, E. F., & Doyle, J. M. (1997). Eyewitness Testimony: Civil and Criminal. LexisNexis.
    • A comprehensive guide for legal professionals on how memory affects witness testimony, with practical applications for attorneys and judges.
  • Schacter, D. L. (2001). The Seven Sins of Memory: How the Mind Forgets and Remembers. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
    • An accessible exploration of memory failures organized into seven categories, building on research by Loftus and others with everyday examples.
  • Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1995). Jeopardy in the Courtroom: A Scientific Analysis of Children’s Testimony. American Psychological Association.
    • Examines how suggestibility affects children’s testimony, with implications for interviewing techniques and legal procedures.

Recommended Websites

  • The Innocence Project
    • Features case studies of wrongful convictions overturned through DNA evidence, many involving faulty eyewitness testimony. Includes educational resources on memory reliability and judicial reform.
  • Elizabeth Loftus Faculty Page at University of California, Irvine
    • Provides access to Loftus’s current research, publications, and media appearances. Includes downloadable articles and videos of her lectures on memory distortion.
  • American Psychological Association – Memory and Trauma
    • Offers evidence-based resources for practitioners on memory reliability, trauma, and therapeutic techniques. Includes professional guidelines for avoiding suggestive practices when working with memory.

Download this Article as a PDF

Download this article as a PDF so you can revisit it whenever you want. We’ll email you a download link.

You’ll also get notification of our FREE Early Years TV videos each week and our exclusive special offers.

Free Article Download

To cite this article use: Loftus and Palmer 1974 Car Crash Experiment. Available at: https://www.earlyyears.tv/loftus-and-palmer-1974-car-crash-experiment (Accessed: 21 May 2025).

Kathy Brodie

Kathy Brodie is an Early Years Professional, Trainer and Author of multiple books on Early Years Education and Child Development. She is the founder of Early Years TV and the Early Years Summit.

Kathy’s Author Profile
Kathy Brodie